|
Indian Pediatr 2009;46: 125-126 |
 |
Iron Supplementation for Improving Mental
Development |
Tarun Gera and *HPS Sachdev
Consultant Pediatrics, SL Jain Hospital, Ashok Vihar
Phase III, New Delhi 110 052;
*Senior Consultant Pediatrics and Clinical Epidemiology,Sitaram Bhartia
Institute of Science and Research,
B-16 Qutab Institutional Area, New Delhi 110 016, India.
E-mail: [email protected] |
I n the absence of a simple, reliable
and cheap indicator of iron status, anemia is invariably used to quantify
the burden of this micronutrient deficiency in public health settings.
Anemia is a major public health problem, particularly in the developing
countries where nearly two billion individuals are affected, with a
significant proportion being constituted by children and women of
childbearing age(1). The etiology of anemia is multifactorial; however,
from a public health perspective, iron deficiency is believed to be the
most important causal factor. The World Health Organization estimates that
roughly 50% of anemia prevalence can be attributed to iron deficiency(1).
Evidence links iron deficiency to several functional consequences; an
important one of these is mental and motor development in children.
Animal studies have provided a number of possible
mechanisms through which iron deficiency can leave an imprint on the
developing brain(2). Most observational studies in children have found
associations between iron deficiency anemia and poor cognitive and motor
development, and behavioral problems(3). Longitudinal studies consistently
indicate that children who were anemic in infancy continue to have poorer
cognition, school achievement, and more behaviour problems into middle
childhood(4). However, the possible confounding effects of environmental
factors, particularly poor socio economic background, prevent causal
inferences from being made.
Sen, et al.(5) present evidence from a
controlled intervention trial that iron and folic acid supplementation in
children aged between 9 and 13 years leads to modest (1.5 to 2 units on a
scale of 100) but significant improvement in the various cognitive tests.
The benefits were greater in anemic subjects, those with higher hemoglobin
increments and with better compliance, and with increasing frequency of
supplementation. However, certain methodological and analytical issues
need consideration while interpreting the findings. The authors did not
rigorously evaluate important confounders of cognitive development
(socio-economic status, parental educational background and slum
conditions). They selected ‘comparable’ schools instead of evaluating
these factors at an individual level. Unfortunately, the school setting
was not exploited to minimize the ‘compliance effect’ by supervised
administration of the intervention. This cluster randomized trial did not
account for the design effect in analyses. The trial appears to be
underpowered to detect a change of 1.5 to 2 units in the cognitive scores.
Finally, after deciding on a specified sample size it is unclear why the
cognitive testing was restricted to a random sub-sample comprising 60% of
the participants. In view of these caveats, the conclusions cannot be
qualified as robust.
Nevertheless, the findings of this trial are in
consonance with the conclusions of a systematic review, which synthesized
data from 17 randomized controlled trials(6). The review concluded that
iron supplementation improves the ‘mental development score’ of children
significantly but modestly (about 0.30 SD units, roughly equivalent to
1.5–2 units on a scale of 100) ; the benefits were greater in initially
anemic or iron deficient subjects, with longer duration of supplementation
(>1 month) and in older children (>2years). However, it would be prudent
to recognize that the systematic review pertained to iron supplementation
alone whereas the intervention in this trial comprised iron and
folic acid. This raises the issue of relative contribution of iron and
folic acid towards the documented improvement in hemoglobin and cognition.
In contrast to iron deficiency, there is scant
information on the magnitude of folate deficiency and its contribution to
the prevalence of anemia and poor neurocognitive development. Even the
choice of intervention in national anemia control program, namely a
combination of iron and folic acid in preference to iron supplementation
alone, is based more on faith rather than sound scientific evidence.
Conversely, a theoretical risk of neurological damage is recognized if
folic acid is administered in the backdrop of undiagnosed vitamin B 12
deficiency. There is negligible evidence about the cognitive benefits of
folate administration from trials in children. However, recent
meta-analyses have documented the effect of folic acid with or without B12
supplementation on cognition and dementia in adults(7,8). Pooled analyses
from four randomized controlled trials showed no effect of folic acid
administration on cognition or dementia, although there was lowering of
homocysteine levels. Thus, the available evidence does not support a clear
role of folate in improving cognition. It would therefore be reasonable to
ascribe the improvement in cognition scores primarily to iron
supplementation in the trial being commented upon(5).
In conclusion, notwithstanding the outlined caveats,
this trial reaffirms the role of iron supplementation in improving
cognition in older children, especially those who are anemic. The
potential reversibility of cognitive deficit with supplementation lends
support to advocacy for public health programs to control iron deficiency.
Funding: None.
Competing interests: None stated.
References
1. WHO/UNICEF/UNU. Iron deficiency anaemia: assessment,
prevention, and control. Geneva, World Health Organization, 2001
(WHO/NHD/01.3). Available from:
http://www.who.int/nut/documents/ida_assessment_prevention_ control.pdf.
Accessed Dec 27, 2008.
2. Beard JL, Connor JR, Jones BC. Iron in the brain.
Nutr Rev 1993; 51: 157-170.
3. Lansdown R, Wharton BA. Iron and mental and motor
behaviour in children. In: Iron, Nutrition and Physiological Significance:
Report of the British Nutrition Task Force. London: Chapman and Hall;
1995. p. 65-78.
4. Grantham-McGregor S, Ani C. A review of studies on
the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children. J Nutr
2001; 131: 649S-668S.
5. Sen A, Kanani SJ. Impact of iron folic acid
supplementation on cognitive abilities of school girls in Vadodara. Indian
Pediatr 2009; 46: 137-143.
6. Sachdev HPS, Gera T, Nestel P. Effect of iron
supplementation on mental and motor development in children: systematic
review of randomized controlled trials. Public Health Nutr 2005; 8:
117-132.
7. Malouf M, Grimley EJ, Areosa SA. Folic acid with or
without vitamin B12 for cognition and dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev
2003; 4: CD004514.
8. Balk EM, Raman G, Tatsioni A, Chung M, Lau J, Rosenberg IH. Vitamin
B6, B12, and folic acid supplementation and cognitive function: a
systematic review of randomized trials. Arch Intern Med 2007; 167: 21-30. |
|
 |
|